Cooperation in multiple object tracking: A Pilot Study
PDF (Español)
XML (Español)

Keywords

cooperation
dyads
multiple object tracking
personality
multiple measurement methods

How to Cite

Bravo-Chávez, R. I., Ferrari-Diaz, M., Silva-Pereyra, J. F., & Fernández-Harmony, T. (2023). Cooperation in multiple object tracking: A Pilot Study. Psicumex, 13(1), 1–23. https://doi.org/10.36793/psicumex.v13i1.454

Metrics

Abstract

Cooperation has its costs but also its benefits. In a Multiple Object Tracking Task with three different levels of difficulty (4, 5, and 6 targets to track, respectively), participants underwent two conditions: solo performance vs performance of the same individual in a dyad. The study's objective was to explore whether the difference in performance between these two conditions is related to cooperation. Ten university students answered a self-report personality questionnaire (Big Five) and performed the visual tracking task. The dyads were recorded on video to identify communication strategies, problem-solving strategies, and target selection agreements. Dyads showed higher scores and slower reaction times compared to solo performance. Slower reaction times were positively associated with verbal exchanges and division of labor strategies. Agreeableness and extraversion, as measured by the self-report questionnaire, were positively associated with agreement on target selection. Interaction between members of a dyad and their personality traits might be relevant for understanding successful cooperation and its costs.

https://doi.org/10.36793/psicumex.v13i1.454
PDF (Español)
XML (Español)

References

Arrighi, R., Lunardi, R., & Burr, D. (2011). Vision and Audition do not Share Attentional Resources in Sustained Tasks. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, art. 56. https://doi.org/10.3389%2Ffpsyg.2011.00056

Bahrami, B., Olsen, K., Latham, P. E., Roepstorff, A., Rees, G., & Frith, C. D. (2010). Optimally Interacting Minds. Science, 329(5995), 1081-1085. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1185718

Bahrami, B., Olsen, K., Bang, D., Roepstorff, A., Rees, G., & Frith, C. (2012). Together, Slowly but Surely: The Role of Social Interaction and Feedback on the Build-Up of Benefit in Collective Decision-Making. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(1), 3-8. https://psycnet.apa.org/fulltext/2011-23156-001.html

Brennan, S. E., Chen, X., Dickinson, C. A., Neider, M. B., & Zelinsky, G. J. (2008). 'Coordinating Cognition: The Costs and Benefits of Shared Gaze During Collaborative Search'. Cognition, 106(3), 1465-1477. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.05.012

Brennan, A. A., & Enns, J. T. (2015a). When Two Heads are Better than One: Interactive Versus Independent Benefits of Collaborative Cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 22(4), 1076-1082. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0765-4

Brennan, A. A., & Enns, J. T. (2015b). What's in a Friendship? Partner Visibility Supports Cognitive Collaboration between Friends. PloS One, 10(11), e0143469. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0143469

Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., Borgogni, L., & Moreno, J. B. (2001). BFQ: Cuestionario" Big Five". Tea.

Cleveland, A., Schug, M., & Striano, T. (2007). Joint Attention and Object Learning in 5-and 7-Month-Old Infants. Infant and Child Development: An International Journal of Research and Practice, 16(3), 295-306. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.508

Davis, M. H. (1980). A Multidimensional Approach to Individual Differences in Empathy. JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 10, 85, 1-19. https://www.uv.es/~friasnav/Davis_1980.pdf

Drew, T., Horowitz, T. S., & Vogel, E. K. (2013). Swapping or Dropping? Electrophysiological Measures of Difficulty During Multiple Object Tracking. Cognition, 126(2), 213-223. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.10.003

Dunfield, K.; Kuhlmeier, V. A.; O’Connell, L., & Kelley, E. (2011), 'Examining the diversity of prosocial behavior: Helping, sharing, and comforting in infancy', Infancy, 16(3), 227--247. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7078.2010.00041.x

Gondan, M., & Minakata, K. (2016). A Tutorial on Testing the Race Model Inequality. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 78(3), 723-735. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-015-1018-y

Harris, D., Wilson, R., Smith, S., Meder, N., & Vine, S. (2020). Testing the Effects of 3D Multiple Object Tracking Training On Near, Mid and Far Transfer. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, art. 196. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00196

Heyman, R. E., Lorber, M. F., Eddy, J. M., & West, T. V. (2014). Behavioral Observation and Coding. Cambridge University Press.

de Jaegher, H., Di Paolo, E., & Gallagher, S. (2010). Can Social Interaction Constitute Social Cognition? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(10), 441-447. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.06.009

Koriat, A. (2012). When are Two Heads Better than One and Why? Science, 336(6079), 360-362. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1216549

Laughlin, P. R., Bonner, B. L., & Miner, A. G. (2002). Groups Perform Better than the Best Individuals on Letters-to-Numbers Problems. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 88(2), 605-620. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-5978(02)00003-1

Laughlin, P. R., Hatch, E. C., Silver, J. S., & Boh, L. (2006). Groups Perform Better than the Best Individuals on Letters-to-Numbers Problems: Effects of Group Size. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90(4), 644-651. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.90.4.644

Laughlin, P. R., Carey, H. R., & Kerr, N. L. (2008). Group-to-Individual Problem-Solving Transfer. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 11(3), 319--330. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430208090645

Meudell, P. R., Hitch, G. J., & Boyle, M. M. (1995). Collaboration in Recall: Do Pairs of People Cross-Cue Each Other to Produce New Memories? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48(1), 141-152. https://doi.org/10.1080/14640749508401381

Meyerhoff, H., Papenmeier, F., & Huff, M. (2017). Studying Visual Attention Using the Multiple Object Tracking Paradigm: A Tutorial Review. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 79(5), 1255-1274. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-017-1338-1

Neider, M. B., Chen, X., Dickinson, C. A., Brennan, S. E., & Zelinsky, G. J. (2010). Coordinating Spatial Referencing Using Shared Gaze. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17(5), 718-724. https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.17.5.718

Pylyshyn, Z. W., & Storm, R. W. (1988). Tracking Multiple Independent Targets: Evidence for a Parallel Tracking Mechanism. Spatial Vision, 3(3), 179-197. https://doi.org/10.1163/156856888x00122

R Core Team. (2014). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Sharabany, R. (1994). Intimate Friendship Scale: Conceptual Underpinnings, Psychometric Properties and Construct Validity. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 11(3), 449-469. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0265407594113010

Sebanz, N., Bekkering, H., & Knoblich, G. (2006). Joint Action: Bodies and Minds Moving Together. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(2), 70-76. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.12.009

Szymanski, C., Pesquita, A., Brennan, A. A., Perdikis, D., Enns, J. T., Brick, T. R., Müller, V., & Lindenberger. U. (2017). Teams on the Same Wavelength Perform Better: Inter-Brain Phase Synchronization Constitutes a Neural Substrate for Social Facilitation. NeuroImage, 152, 425–436. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.03.013

Van-Rossum, G., & Drake, F. L. (2009). Python 2.6 Reference Manual. Phyton.

Van-Zandt, T. (2002). Analysis of Response Time Distributions. In H. Pashler y J. Wixted (Eds.), Stevens’ Handbook of Experimental Psychology, 4, 461-516. John Wiley & Sons Inc. https://doi.org/10.1002/0471214426.pas0412

Wahn, B., Kingstone, A., & König, P. (2017). Two Trackers Are Better Than One: Information About the Co-Actor’s Actions and Performance Scores Contribute to the Collective Benefit in a Joint Visuospatial Task. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, art. 669. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00669

Wahn, B., & König, P. (2017). Can Limitations of Visuospatial Attention Be Circumvented? A Review. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, art. 1896. https://doi.org/10.3389%2Ffpsyg.2017.01896

Wahn, B., Czeszumski, A., & König, P. (2018). Performance Similarities Predict Collective Benefits in Dyadic and Triadic Joint Visual Search. PloS One, 13(1), artículo e0191179. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0191179

Wahn, B., Karlinsky, A., Schmitz, L., & König, P. (2018). Let's Move It Together: A Review of Group Benefits in Joint Object Control. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, art. 918. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00918

Wahn, B., Kingstone, A., & König, P. (2018). Group Benefits in Joint Perceptual Tasks—A Review. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1426(1), 166-178. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.13843

Wahn, B., Czeszumski, A., Labusch, M., Kingstone, A., & König, P. (2020). Dyadic and Triadic Search: Benefits, Costs, and Predictors of Group Performance. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 82, 2415–2433. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-019-01915-0

Wahn, B., König, P., & Kingston, A. (2020). Predicting Group Benefits in Joint Multiple Object Tracking. PsyArXiv Prepints. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/fzwgs

Creative Commons License

This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.

Copyright (c) 2022

Downloads

Download data is not yet available.