Space conceptualization: Language and cognitive egocentrism
pdf (Español)
XML (Español)

Keywords

Spatial conceptualization, cognitive egocentrism, linguistic construction, intelligence, development.

How to Cite

Calderón Guerrero, G., & Lozada Nava, L. . (2020). Space conceptualization: Language and cognitive egocentrism. Psicumex, 10(1), 75–91. https://doi.org/10.36793/psicumex.v10i1.331

Metrics

Abstract

The conceptualization of space and its communicability to other human beings are essential for human survivial and the genesis and support of culture (Piaget and Inhelder, 2015; Chatterjee, 2001). This study analyzes how the participants solved a task that involves the conceptualization of space, in which it was requested to describe the spatial location of a series of objects to another person. 60 mexican children and adolescents organized in three different age groups (9-10, 11-12 and 13-14 years) were interviewed. The investigation consisted in two phases. In the first one, a diagnostic test of inclusion was carried out to ensure that participants were not disadvantaged in the knowledge of left and rigth locations according to their age. In the second phase the task of “Scene description” was performed. The analysis of the data indicates a systematic difference in the conceptualization of the space between children of different groups, reflected by a statistically significant difference in the variable “type of linguistic construction”. The findings make it possible to verify that the descriptions of the participants are increasingly precise, since they provide more and better information to other person. This work also provides cualitative information. Finally, this study show the usefulness of linguistic analysis to describe, to quantify and to study  the conceptualization of space and its relation with the overcoming of cognitive egocentrism, that is, it is usefulness in analyzing the development of intelligence.

https://doi.org/10.36793/psicumex.v10i1.331
pdf (Español)
XML (Español)

References

Blythe, J., Mardigan, K., Perdjert, M., y Stoakes, H. (2016). Pointing out directions in Murrinhpatha. Open Linguistics, 2(open-issue), 132-159.

Calderón, E., De Pascale, S., y Adamou, E. (2019). How to speak “geocentric” in an “egocentric” language: A multimodal study among Ngigua-Spanish bilinguals and Spanish monolinguals in a rural community of Mexico. Language Sciences, 74, 24-46.

Calderón, G. (2015). La metáfora como recurso para comprender la mente infantil. Fontamara/UAQ.

Calsamiglia, H., y Tusón, A. (2018). Las cosas del decir: manual de análisis del discurso. Ariel.

Chatterjee, A. (2001). Language and space: some interactions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(2), 55-61. https//doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01598-9

Craik, F., y Lockhart, R. (1972). Levels of proccessing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, (12), 599-607.

Cassany, D. (2019). Laboratorio lector para entender la lectura. Anagrama.

Dasen, P., Changkakoti, N., Abbiati, M., Niraula, S., Mishra, R., y Foy, H. (2009). Geocentric gestures as a research tool. En A. Gari, y K. Mylonas (Eds.), Quod Erat Demonstrandum: From Herodotus’ ethnographic journeys to cross-cultural research (115-121). Pedio Books.

Ferreiro, E., y García, E. (1987). Presentación de la edición castellana. En J. Piaget (Ed.), Introducción a la epistemología genética. El pensamiento matemático (9-23). Paidós Psicología Evolutiva.

Goeke, C., Kornpetpanee, S., Köster, M., Fernández-Revelles, A. B., Gramann, K., y König, P. (2015). Cultural background shapes spatial reference frame proclivity. Scientific reports, 5, 11426. https://www.nature.com/articles/srep11426

Hegarty, M., Keehner, M., Cohen, C., Montello, D., y Lippa, Y. (2007). The Role of Spatial Cognition in Medicine: Applications for Selecting and Training Professionals. En G. L. Allen (Ed.), Applied spatial cognition: From research to cognitive technology (285-315). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

Ibarretxe-Antuñano, I. (2008). ¿Influye la lengua que hablamos en nuestra conceptualización del espacio? El caso de los marcos de referencia espaciales. Ciencia Cognitiva: Revista Electrónica de Divulgación, 2(1), 10-12.

Lanca, M. (1998). Three-dimensional representations of contour maps. Contemporary educational psychology, 23(1), 22-41.

Levinson, S. (1996). Language and space. Annual review of Anthropology, 25(1), 353-382. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev. anthro.25.1.353

Levinson, S. (2003). Space in Language and Cognition: Explorations in Cognitive Diversity. Cambridge Univ. Press.

Levinson, S., y Wilkins, D. (2006). Grammars of Space. Explorations in Cognitive Diversity. Cambridge University Press.

Lin, C., Chiu, C., y Gramann, K. (2015). EEG correlates of spatial orientation in the human retrosplenial complex. NeuroImage, 120, 123-132.

López, F., Sánchez, E., y Jiménez, F. (1988). Egocentrismo especial: concepto y procesos implicados. Infancia y aprendizaje, (43), 97- 122.

Lurcat, L. (1979). El niño y el espacio. La función del cuerpo. FCE.

Mares, G. (2001). Transferencia desde una perspectiva de desarrollo psicológico. En G. Mares, y Y. Guevara (Eds.), Psicología interconductual. Avances en la investigación básica. Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México.

Matute, E., Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., y Ostrosky, F. (2013). Evaluación Neuropsicológica Infantil (ENI-2). Ed. El Manual Moderno.

Muñoz, R., y Alonqueo, P. (2017). Referencias espaciales lingüísticas y deícticas de niños rurales mapuches y no mapuches: un estudio exploratorio sobre la descripción del trayecto en espacios amplios. Revista de Lingüística Teórica y Aplicada, 55(1), 73-94.

Palmer, B., Lum, J., Schlossberg, J., y Gaby, A. (2017). How does the environment shape spatial language? Evidence for sociotopography. Linguistic Typology, 21(3), 457-491.

Pavez, M., Coloma, C., y Maggiollo, M. (2008). El desarrollo narrativo en niños. Ars Médica.

Pavez, M., Coloma, C., Maggiolo, M., Martínez, L., y Romero, L. (2002). Procedimientos para evaluar discurso (PREDI). Ediciones Universidad Católica de Chile.

Pederson, E., Danziger, E., Wilkins, D., Levinson, S., Kita, S., y Senft, G. (1998). Semantic typology and spatial conceptualization. Language, 74(3), 557-589. 10.2307/417793

Piaget, J. (1989). Seis estudios. Ariel.

Piaget, J. (2012). La equilibración de las estructuras cognitivas. Siglo XXI.

Piaget, J. (2013). Egocentric thought and sociocentric thought. En L. Smith (Ed.), Sociological studies (276-286). Routledge.

Piaget, J., y Inhelder, B. (1947). The child´s conception of space. Routledge y Keegan Paul.

Piaget, J., y Inhelder, B. (2015). Psicología del niño. Morata.

Pozo, J. (1997). Teorías cognitivas del aprendizaje. Morata.

Rimassa, C., y Fernández-Silva, S. (2014). Conceptualización del espacio y su relación con el desarrollo cognitivo: un estudio piloto en el español de Chile. Alpha, (38), 137-154. http://dx.doi.org/10.4067/S0718- 22012014000100010

Shepard, R. N., y Hurwitz, S. (1984). Upward direction, mental rotation, and discrimination of left and right turns in maps. Cognition, 18(1-3), 161-193.

Tommasi, L., y Laeng, B. (2012). Psychology of spatial cognition. Widley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 3(6), 565-580. 10.1002/wcs.1198

Vigostky, L. (2010). Pensamiento y lenguaje. Paidós.

Werlen, B. (2017). Action, Knowledge, and Social Relations of Space. En P. Meusburger, B. Werlen, y L. Suarsana (Eds.), Knowledge and Action. Knowledge and Space, 9, 31-56. Springer Open.

Wu, B., Klatzky, R., y Stetten, G. (2010). Visualizing 3D objects from 2D cross sectional images displayed in-situ versus exsitu. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 16(1), 45-60.

Creative Commons License

This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.

Copyright (c) 2020

Downloads

Download data is not yet available.